传媒教育网

 找回密码
 实名注册

QQ登录

只需一步,快速开始

搜索
做个试验
楼主: admin
打印 上一主题 下一主题

新闻语言的语用案例

[复制链接]
231#
 楼主| 发表于 2012-4-24 18:53:34 | 只看该作者
【案例】
马九器我听出弦外之音啦
@王兴律师在重庆坐车真好玩,一会上山,一会入地,真是过山车啊
收起|查看大圖|向左轉|向右轉

轉發(2)
|
評論(1)
4分鐘前
來自Android客户端
轉發|
收藏|
評論
2分鐘前
來自新浪微博
232#
 楼主| 发表于 2012-4-27 17:43:37 | 只看该作者
【案例】
美联社发布最新2012年新闻用语词典封面

2012年04月27日14:50 新浪传媒

美联社发布最新2012年新闻用语词典封面

  新浪传媒讯:据美国“波因特学院”网站4月25日报道,美联社在其Facebook页面上这样写道:“我们已在今天审批通过了2012年新闻用语词典AP Stylebook校稿,接下来就是等待印刷了”。新的Stylebook封面展示出了一种庄重霓虹灯状的紫色背景,反射出了新服务的新图标形状。
  美联社品牌和创意服务总监Tina Antonion设计了这个封面,她说,紫色对新闻服务来说是一种“全新的境界”。在标记的再次设计中,公司的设计师们使用了最新的“深度”调色板,包括庄重的紫色和“亮色”,以及那些线条。她说,这些东西的目的是要表现一种“运动的感觉”,与标记的连接代表美联社“与我们的资源相连,与我们的客户相连”。
  新标记下的红色线条突出了封面上的文本,Antonion称,此处的红色线条是为了“强调内容的重要性”。那紫色呢?她说:“我们想将更多的颜色引入编辑室”。深浅同色的封面因她称之为“特例”被保留了下来,就像这本新的Stylebook一样。
  Stylebook的价格将上涨1美元,最终价格为20.95美元。(秦朗)

http://news.sina.com.cn/m/2012-04-27/145024340395.shtml

233#
 楼主| 发表于 2012-4-28 15:48:21 | 只看该作者
【案例】
中國時報王銘義//@铁木不真:雄文啊,必讀!//@申音: 发改委发审委网管办都是天朝语文
@蛮子文摘#好文推荐#
《张维迎:语言腐败在中国已到无以复加地步》(推荐达人@申音
收起|查看大圖|向左轉|向右轉

轉發(1948)
|
評論(381)
今天11:50
來自新浪微博
轉發|
收藏|
評論
10分鐘前
來自iPhone客户端
234#
 楼主| 发表于 2012-4-30 10:20:24 | 只看该作者
【案例】
北大新传徐泓有人能否提供其它国家外交辞令之比较吗?
@王强_99[url=http://weibo.com/verify][/url]:《外交部发言用词解析》 1、亲切友好的交谈——谈的不错;2、坦率交谈——分歧很大,无法沟通;3、交换了意见——基本各说各的,没有达成协议;4、双方充分交换了意见——双方吵得厉害;5、增进了双方的了解——分歧很大;6、严重关切——可能要干预,但很可能歇菜;7表示极大愤慨——拿人家真没辙!



轉發(3860) | 評論(562) 4月29日23:25 來自iPhone客户端
轉發(28)| 收藏| 評論(10) 18分鐘前 來自新浪微博
235#
 楼主| 发表于 2012-4-30 16:52:55 | 只看该作者
【案例】
环球舆情-戴元初:The Effects of Semantics and Social Desirability。 //@环球舆情-戴元初:the effective of semantics。效果研究的深度。
◆◆@密苏里大学新闻学院孙志刚博士[url=http://weibo.com/verify][/url]:当人们以为你是A,你说你不是A;当人们以为你是A,你说你是B。出于同样目的,前者用了否定句,后者用了肯定句。就澄清自己/说服别人而言,哪个更有效?美国学者Brendan Nyhan 和 Jason Reifler(2009年)通过科研表明:后者更有效,同时提出“适得其反之效果”Backfire Effect的观点http://t.cn/zOY8JDv 轉發(50) | 評論(10) 今天07:22 來自新浪微博
轉發| 收藏| 評論 16分鐘前 來自新浪微博

Behind the News, Regret the Error — June 17, 2011 11:44 AM The Backfire Effect
More on the press’s inability to debunk bad information
TAGS: backfire effect, Barack Obama, Brendan Nyhan, Jason Reifler, misinformation, Regret the Error


Which of these headlines strikes you as the most persuasive:
“I am not a Muslim, Obama says.”
“I am a Christian, Obama says.”
The first headline is a direct and unequivocal denial of a piece of misinformation that’s had a frustratingly long life. It’s Obama directly addressing the falsehood.
The second option takes a different approach by affirming Obama’s true religion, rather than denying the incorrect one. He’s asserting, not correcting.
Which one is better at convincing people of Obama’s religion? According to recent research into political misinformation, it’s likely the latter.
The study was led by Brendan Nyhan and Jason Reifler, two leading researchers examining political misinformation and the ways in which it can and can’t be refuted, among other topics. Their 2009 paper, “The Effects of Semantics and Social Desirability in Correcting the Obama Muslim Myth,” found that affirming statements appeared to be more effective at convincing people to abandon or question their incorrect views regarding President Obama’s religion.
I found their work courtesy of an exhaustive post on You Are Not So Smart, a blog about “self delusion and irrational thinking” by journalist David McRaney.
McRaney spends several thousand words explaining the “backfire effect,” which he nicely summarized in one sentence: “When your deepest convictions are challenged by contradictory evidence, your beliefs get stronger.”
As I detailed in a recent column, the backfire effect makes it difficult for the press to effectively debunk misinformation. We present facts and evidence, and it often does nothing to change people’s minds. In fact, it can make people dig in even more. Humans also engage in motivated reasoning, a tendency to let emotions “set us on a course of thinking that’s highly biased, especially on topics we care a great deal about”.
These two important cognitive effects can have a significant impact on society and debates in the public sphere. They also end up negating some of the debunking and reporting work done by the press. My recent attempts to understand the backfire effect and motivated reasoning has transformed into a search for ways to combat these entrenched human phenomena.
I sought out Reifler, an assistant professor of political science at Georgia State University, to learn more about his and his colleagues’ findings regarding affirmative statements and their effect of the Obama Muslim myth. I asked him if there are other other ways of presenting information that can debunk lies.
“I’m sure that there are but I don’t know what they are,” he told me, ever the cautious researcher.
Nevertheless, he did offer some encouragement.
“I think we’re moving in that direction,” he says.
Part of the process of discovering what works is to rule out what doesn’t. I listed a some of them in my previous column, and Nyhan and Reifler provide more evidence in a 2010 paper, “When Corrections Fail: The Persistence of Political Misperceptions,” published in Political Behavior. (Note that their definition of a correction is different from the ones used in the press.) Their study saw respondents read a mock news article “containing a statement from a political figure that reinforces a widespread misperception.” Some of the articles also included a paragraph of text that refuted (or “corrected”) the misperception and statement.
One article, for example, led with President George W. Bush talking about Iraq and the possibility it “would pass weapons or materials or information to terrorist networks.” It then transitioned to a graph that cited information from a CIA report that Iraq did not in fact possess illicit weapons at the time of the U.S.-led invasion. Would these corrective paragraphs influence respondents who believed Iraq had WMDs?
As the researches write, the corrective sections “frequently fail to reduce misperceptions among the targeted ideological group.”
Then there’s that familiar term: “We also document several instances of a ‘backfire effect’ in which corrections actually increase misperceptions among the group in question.”
So perhaps a single, credible refutation within a news article isn’t likely to convince people to change their views. But other research suggests that a constant flow of these kind of corrections could help combat misinformation. The theory is that the more frequently someone is exposed to information that goes against their incorrect beliefs, the more likely it is that they will change their views.
“It’s possible there is something to be said for persistence,” Reifler said. “At some point the cost of always being wrong or always getting information that runs counter to what you believe is likely to outweigh the cost of having to change your mind about something. We need to figure out what is the magic breaking or tipping point, or what leads people to get to that tipping point. I think we’re just scratching the surface.”
He pointed to a 2010 paper in Political Psychology by David P. Redlawsk and others, “The Affective Tipping Point: Do Motivated Reasoners Ever ‘Get It’?”
The researchers sought to determine if a tipping point exists that could cause voters to abandon motivated reasoning and view facts in a more rational way.
“We show experimental evidence that such an affective tipping point does in fact exist,” they write. “… The existence of a tipping point suggests that voters are not immune to disconfirming information after all, even when initially acting as motivated reasoners.”
This tipping point is far from being identified, but it’s encouraging to think that repeated efforts to debunk misinformation, or to simply to spread the truth, may have an effect.
One final cause for hope is that Reifler and Nyhan are conducting studies to see if the visual presentation of information can impact its level of persuasion. As none of this work has been finalized, Reifler declined to share details on the record. But the overall point is that after decades of research that has demonstrated the human propensity for motivated reasoning and the backfire effect, researchers are moving towards identifying keys that can unlock our closed minds.
Good news, right? Not exactly, according to Reifler.
He said researchers first began looking at these forms of persuasion after World War II in order to understand how Nazism could persuade millions of people. As a result, researchers were initially encouraged to discover the human resistance to persuasion.
“The difficulty of persuading people was seen as good thing,” Reifler said. “It meant that it would be more difficult for really, really terrible things to happen. Anytime we’re talking about persuasion or getting people to change their beliefs there is always a good side and a dark side.”
Correction of the Week
An extract of an online opinion piece appeared in the newspaper, headlined Will and Kate’s mask slips (9 June, page 31). It argued that while, pre-wedding, it was announced that the future Duke and Duchess of Cambridge would not be employing household staff, this image of modernity had now been “compromised by the news that they are advertising for a housekeeper, butler, valet and dresser to serve them in their new home of Kensington Palace”. The couple’s press secretary, Miguel Head, asks us to make clear that: “At most, they may employ one (a cleaner-cum-housekeeper), who may be part-time. We never ‘announced’ that the couple would ‘not be employing any [domestic staff]‘ after their wedding. What we have always said is that the couple have no plans to employ domestic staff at their home in Anglesey, but in London they have use of domestic staff at Clarence House, the home that they have hitherto shared with the Prince of Wales. The additional one part-time, or one full-time, cleaner has come about because the couple are taking their own home in London away from Clarence House.” Elsewhere the piece referred to “damaging stories of royal profligacy past: Charles with his staff of 150, and an aide to squeeze his toothpaste for him”. Of this, Miguel Head writes: “The Prince of Wales does not employ and has never employed an aide to squeeze his toothpaste for him. This is a myth without any basis in factual accuracy.” - The Guardian

http://www.cjr.org/behind_the_news/the_backfire_effect.php?page=all

236#
 楼主| 发表于 2012-5-4 21:30:46 | 只看该作者
【案例】
慎独小猪怎么赢?
收起|查看大圖|向左轉|向右轉

轉發(3)|
收藏|
評論(1)
9分鐘前
來自iPhone客户端
237#
 楼主| 发表于 2012-5-6 21:39:13 | 只看该作者
【案例】
@焦点新闻社据调查,中国40岁以上官员中,超过80%与原配常年没有性生活,他们又不准备离婚。老百姓亲切地把这种现象称为:一不做,二不休
收起|查看大圖|向左轉|向右轉

轉發(288)
|
評論(47)
今天09:45
來自皮皮时光机
238#
 楼主| 发表于 2012-5-7 21:46:27 | 只看该作者
【案例】字
黃海波白勺石角女少
@胡洪侠乱逛博客,见有如此“拆字奇文”:“不 矢口 亻十 么 日寸 候 , 亻奄 口斤 言兑 言仑 土云 有 辶寸 氵虑 敏 感 字节白勺 言兑 氵去 , 于 是 , 亻奄 学 会 了 扌斥 字 , 后 来 , 亻奄 米青 礻申 分 歹刂 了。”汉字之妙,真无所不在。轉發(13)
|
評論(4)
23分鐘前
來自新浪微博
轉發(8)|
收藏|
評論(4)
17分鐘前
來自新浪微博
239#
 楼主| 发表于 2012-5-8 23:51:13 | 只看该作者
本帖最后由 admin 于 2012-5-8 23:52 编辑

【案例】
张默然动画
//@怎么验2012://@青媒素: 当年就知道菲律宾觊觎黄岩岛。
@冬的颜色当年谁发明了扫黄打非这词儿?真有想法!轉發(51)
|
評論(15)
17分鐘前
來自iPhone客户端
轉發|
收藏|
評論(1)
6分鐘前
來自iPhone客户端

闲情偶记
//@京都狂鬼: //@陆天明: 对。扫黄,打菲。打。打。 打。也得出一口气了。挺一回腰杆子吧!!
@闲情偶记【扫黄打菲】黄岩岛主权是中华人民共和国的,马尼拉主权是菲律宾共和国的。菲律宾共和国到黄岩岛来清扫,中华人民共和国到马尼拉主权去清扫,道理完全是一样的——你“扫黄”,我“打菲”,只是礼尚往来,连义务劳动都算不上!
收起|查看大圖|向左轉|向右轉

轉發(127)
|
評論(30)
今天21:25
來自皮皮时光机
轉發(4)|
收藏|
評論(1)
8分鐘前
來自iPad客户端
240#
 楼主| 发表于 2012-5-13 23:48:25 | 只看该作者
【案例】
传媒老王发微博 用汉语最佳
收起|查看大圖|向左轉|向右轉

轉發(2)|
收藏|
評論(2)
5分鐘前
來自iPad客户端

发表回复

您需要登录后才可以回帖 登录 | 实名注册

本版积分规则

掌上论坛|小黑屋|传媒教育网 ( 蜀ICP备16019560号-1

Copyright 2013 小马版权所有 All Rights Reserved.

Powered by Discuz! X3.2

© 2016-2022 Comsenz Inc.

快速回复 返回顶部 返回列表